Human Eye Beats Machine in Archaeological Colour Identification Test

A ruler and scale can tell archaeologists the size and weight of a fragment of pottery – but identifying its precise colour can depend on individual perception. So, when a handheld colour-matching gadget came on the market, scientists hoped it offered a consistent way of determining colour, free of human bias.

But a new study by archaeologists at the Florida Museum of Natural History found that the tool, known as the X-Rite Capsure, often misread colours readily distinguished by the human eye.

When tested against a book of colour chips, the machine failed to produce correct colour scores in 37.5% of cases, even though its software system included the same set of chips. In an analysis of fired clay bricks, the Capsure matched archaeologists’ colour scores only 35% of the time, dropping to about 5% matching scores when reading sediment colours in the field. Researchers also found the machine was prone to reading colour chips as more yellow than they were and sediment and clay as too red.

 

“I think that we were surprised by how much we disagreed with the instrument. We had the expectation that it would kind of act as the moderator and resolve conflicts,” said Lindsay Bloch, collection manager of the Florida Museum’s Ceramic Technology Lab and lead study author. “Instead, the device would often have an entirely different answer that we all agreed was wrong.”

Identifying subtle differences in colour can help archaeologists compare the composition of soil and the origins of artefacts, such as pottery and beads, to understand how people lived and interacted in the past. Colour can also reveal whether materials have been exposed to fire, indicating how communities used surrounding natural resources.

Today, the Munsell colour system, created by Albert Munsell in 1905 and later adopted by the U.S. Department of Agriculture for soil research, is the archaeological standard for identifying colours. Researchers use a binder of 436 unique colour chips to determine a Munsell colour score for artifacts, sediment and objects such as bones, shell and rocks. These scores enable archaeologists around the world to compare colours across sites and time periods. But the process of assigning scores can vary based on lighting conditions, the quality of a sample and the perspective of the researcher.

This study is the first to test and record the accuracy of the X-Rite Capsure, a device made by the same company that owns the colour authority Pantone. Although marketed to archaeologists, the device was originally designed for interior designers and cosmetologists, not research, Bloch said.

 

“I think the main takeaway was just sort of surprise that it’s something that is marketed for our field, specifically for archaeologists, but hasn’t been made for us and the kind of data we need to collect,” she added. “When you read the manual, it says you should always verify that the colour the machine tells you looks right with your eyes, which seems to negate the use of the instrument.”

In an experiment designed with the help of University of Florida undergraduate researchers Claudette Lopez and Emily Kracht, the team tested the Capsure’s readings of the three elements of Munsell’s system: a colour’s general family, or hue; intensity, also known as chroma; and lightness, also called value.

The team first tested the Capsure on all 436 Munsell soil colour chips, rating its reading as correct if it matched the exact score on a chip three out of five times. It correctly scored 274 chips. Of its errant readings, about 75% were misidentifications of hue. The Capsure was consistent, though often wrong, producing the same reading five times for 89% of the chips.

To determine how well the machine performed in a typical laboratory setting, the team tested its colour readings of 140 pottery briquettes that had been assigned Munsell scores by Lopez. The Capsure matched the archaeologist’s scores in 35% of cases, again tending to misread hue. It proved consistent in this second test as well, yielding the same score across all trials of more than 70% of the briquettes.

In the most challenging of colour-identification conditions – outdoors, where lighting and texture can vary – the machine only matched archaeologists’ scores of sediment samples about 5% of the time, often rating a shade darker or lighter. For one sample, the Capsure reported colours from five different families, even though archaeologists agreed the sediment was a single hue. Bloch said the discrepancy was likely due to moisture, sand and shells, which don’t usually interfere with human observations.

Unlike some other methods of identifying colour, the Capsure is a remote control-sized device that can provide a reading in seconds. Bloch said the tool’s simple design and accessibility lend it to other scientific applications, but that the team’s results point to a need for further scrutiny of how archaeologists record colour.

“This new tool has really forced us to see that colour is subjective and that, even with a supposedly objective instrument, it may be much more complicated than we’ve been led to believe,” she said. “We need to pay really close attention and record how we’re describing colour in order to make good data. Ultimately, if we’re putting bad colour data in, we’re going to get bad data out.”

Bloch said she would give the Capsure three out of five stars for being easy to use and offering helpful ways to store data.

“The ding is for the quality of data because it’s still kind of unknown. At this point, I think that our team would say the subjective eye is better.”

FLORIDA MUSEUM OF NATURAL HISTORY

Header Image Credit : Lindsay Bloch/Florida Museum

Download the HeritageDaily mobile application on iOS and Android

More on this topic

Markus Milligan
Markus Milliganhttps://www.heritagedaily.com
Markus Milligan - Markus is a journalist and the Managing Editor at HeritageDaily. His background is in archaeology and computer science, having written over 7,000 articles across several online publications. Markus is a member of the Association of British Science Writers (ABSW).
spot_img

LATEST NEWS

spot_img

Popular stories

WHO WE SUPPORT